We are unable to display your institutional affiliation without JavaScript turned on.
Browse Book and Journal Content on Project MUSE
OR

Find using OpenURL

Rent from DeepDyve Rent from DeepDyve

The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation

From: Language
Volume 53, Number 2, June 1977
pp. 361-382 | 10.1353/lan.1977.0041

In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:

[Received 27 July 1976.]

References

BOLINGER , DWIGHT L. 1953. The life and death of words. American Scholar 22.323-35. [Reprinted in his Forms of English: accent, morpheme, order. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1965.]
CARTERETTE , EDWARD C., and MARGARET HUBBARD JONES . 1974. Informal speech: alphabetic and phonemic texts with statistical analyses and tables. Berkeley & Los Angeles: University of California Press.
DU BOIS , JOHN W. 1974. Syntax in mid-sentence. Berkeley studies in syntax and semantics, I, ed. by Charles Fillmore, George Lakoff, & Robin Lakoff, pp. III.1-25. Berkeley: Department of Linguistics and Institute of Human Learning, University of California.
FROMKIN , VICTORIA A. (ed.) 1973. Speech errors as linguistic evidence. The Hague: Mouton.
FRY , D. B. 1969. The linguistic evidence of speech errors. Brno Studies in English 8.70-74. [Reprinted in Fromkin, pp. 157-63.]
GARVEY , CATHERINE . 1975. Contingent queries. MS . Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University, Department of Psychology.
HOCKETT , C. F. 1967. Where the tongue slips, there slip I. To honor Roman Jakobson, 910-36. The Hague, Mouton. [Reprinted in Fromkin, pp. 93-119.]
JEFFERSON , GAIL . 1972. Side sequences. Studies in social interaction, ed. by David N. Sudnow, 294-338. New York: Free Press.
———. 1973. A case of precision timing in ordinary conversation. Semiotica 9.47-96.
———. 1975. Error correction as an interactional resource. Language in Society 3.181-99.
JORDAN , B., and N. FULLER . 1975. The non-fatal nature of trouble: sense-making and trouble managing in lingua franca talk. Semiotica 13.11-31.
LABOV , WILLIAM . 1970. The study of language in its social context. Studium Generale 23.30-87.
LAVER , JOHN D. M. 1969. The detection and correction of slips of the tongue. Work in Progress 3, Dept. of Phonetics and Linguistics, University of Edinburgh. [Reprinted in Fromkin, pp. 132-43.]
PARSONS , TALCOTT . 1937. The structure of social action. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
POMERANTZ , ANITA . 1975. Second assessments: a study of some features of agreements/disagreements. Irvine: University of California dissertation.
SACKS , H., and E. SCHEGLOFF . 1977. Two preferences in the organization of reference to persons in conversation and their interaction. To appear in Papers from the Boston University Conference on Ethnomethodology, ed. by George Psathas.
SACKS , H.; E. SCHEGLOFF ; and G. JEFFERSON . 1974. A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Lg. 50.696-735.
SCHEGLOFF , E. 1968. Sequencing in conversational openings. American Anthropologist 70.1075-95.
———. 1976. Some questions and ambiguities in conversation. Cambridge, England: Pragmatics Microfiche.
———, and H. SACKS . 1973. Opening up closings. Semiotica 8.289-327.

Footnotes

* We wish to acknowledge the help, through discussion and/or through bringing relevant data to our attention, of Jo Ann Goldberg, Anita Pomerantz, and Alene Terasaki at the University of California, Irvine, and of Françoise Brun-Cottan, Irene Daden, and Louise Kerr at the University of California, Los Angeles. Harvey Sacks was killed in an automobile accident while this paper was undergoing final revision.

1. Bolinger ([1953] 1965:248) writes:

'What speakers avoid doing is as important as what they do. Self-correction of speech and writing, and the correction of others in conversation ("I can't understand what you say"), in classrooms, and over editorial desks is an unending business, one that determines the outlines of our speech just as acceptances determine its mass. Correction, the border beyond which we say "no" to an expression, is to language what a seacoast is to a map. Up to now, linguistic scientists have ignored it because they could see in it nothing more than the hankerings of pedants after a standard that is arbitrary, prejudiced and personal. But it goes deeper. Its motive is intelligibility, and in spite of the occasional aberrations that have distracted investigators from the central facts, it is systematic enough to be scientifically described.'

Not much has been made of the distinction—in part, perhaps, because the disciplines have used it to divide up their work, self-correction being occasionally discussed by linguists (since it regularly occurs within the sentence?), e.g. Hockett...



You must be logged in through an institution that subscribes to this journal or book to access the full text.

Shibboleth

Shibboleth authentication is only available to registered institutions.

Project MUSE

For subscribing associations only.