In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content:

  • A Longitudinal Study of Digit Ratio (2D:4D) and Its Relationships with Adult Running Speed in Jamaicans
  • Robert Trivers, Renato Hopp, and John Manning

Dear Editor,

We write to point out that digit ratio may be a useful childhood biomarker for endurance running in adults. We have considered this question in a cohort of Jamaican children who have been studied from 1996 to the present day (Trivers et al. 1999).

Digit ratio (2D:4D; the relative lengths of the second and fourth digits) is a negative correlate of prenatal testosterone (PT) (Manning et al. 2007). PT has organizing effects on many traits, including performance in sport. Low 2D:4D (high PT) is linked to endurance sports, but it is unclear whether 2D:4D measured in children and teenagers is predictive of their performance when they are adults (Manning et al. 2007).

The purpose of this letter is to consider this question in a long-term study (the Jamaican Symmetry Project, JSP) of participants from a rural Jamaican population (Trivers et al. 1999). The JSP consists of a cohort of 288 children (155 boys) that were first measured in 1996 when participants were 5–11 years. The JSP focuses on developmental stability, but many other traits have been included. 2D:4D was measured in 1996 from hand x-rays and also from hand photocopies in 2002, and in 2010 for the present study. These measurements afforded us the opportunity to consider a longitudinal study of childhood, teenage, and adult 2D:4D as predictors of endurance-related adult performance in sport. We examined the participant performance in two races (90 m and 180 m) and focused on participant time for the 180 m relative to time for the 90 m.

We considered the hypothesis that low 2D:4D is linked to endurance and made the following predictions: (1) participants with low 2D:4D would run faster in the 180 m than expected after consideration of their 90-m times—that is, 2D:4D would be positively correlated to 180-m times after the influence of 90-m times was removed (residuals of 180-m times regressed on 90-m times: res180mon90m); and (2) this pattern of relationships not only would be found for 2D:4D measured in 2010 but also would apply to 2D:4D measured in 2002 and 1996.

As our experimental protocol, we ran the first race for each subject (90 m) followed by second race (180 m) about 30 min after the first race. Therefore, any increase in exhaustion from the first race affected the longer, second race, the critical measure being the relative difference between the two. Subjects were run in pairs. Finger measurements were made from scans of the ventral surface of the hand. [End Page 623]


Click for larger view
View full resolution
Table 1.

Mean (SD) for Right and Left 2D:4D for Males and Females Measured in 2010 (n = 160; 97 males), 2001 (n = 130; 80 males), and 1996 (n = 146; 88 males)

Results

Means (SD) for 2D:4D are given in Table 1. With regard to sex differences, as expected there was a tendency for males to have lower 2D:4D compared with females, but this was not significant, and the tendency was not found for 1996. There were significant correlations between the means for 2010 and 2002 (right, r = 0.76; left, r = 0.78) and for 2010 and 1996 (right, r = 0.46; left r = 0.51), all at p < 0.0001. Males ran significantly faster than females for the 90 m (means ± SD: males, 12.97 ± 1.52 s; females, 17.60 ± 2.9 s; t = 11.75; p < 0.0001) and the 180 m (males, 28.26 ± 4.80 s; females, 38.80 ± 5.80 s; t = 12.70; p < 0.0001).

We were interested in each participant’s relative performance in the 180-m run compared with that in the 90-m run. Therefore, we considered res180mon90m and analyzed male and female data separately. We found that 2D:4D was positively related to res180mon90m for all correlations. That is, participants with low 2D:4D tended to run faster in the 180-m race than was expected after consideration...

pdf